Letter to the Editor June 15, 2011

Cognitive Inhibition in Suicidal Depressed Elderly: A Case-Control Pilot Study

Stephane Richard-Devantoy, MD, MS; Cédric Annweiler, MD, MS; Didier Le Gall, PhD; Jean-Bernard Garré, MD, PhD; Jean-Pierre Olié, MD, PhD; Olivier Beauchet, MD, PhD

J Clin Psychiatry 2011;72(6):871-872

Article Abstract

Letter to the Editor

Because this piece does not have an abstract, we have provided for your benefit the first 3 sentences of the full text.

Suicide is defined as the intentional taking of one’s own life. Its prevalence is particularly high among older adults and ranges from 30/100,000 to 120/100,000. Although it is recognized that younger suicidal depressed patients have greater executive dysfunctions than nonsuicidal ones, little remains known about the role of executive functions in elderly suicide attempts

Cognitive Inhibition in Suicidal Depressed Elderly: A Case-Control Pilot Study

To the Editor: Suicide is defined as the intentional taking of one’s own life.1 Its prevalence is particularly high among older adults and ranges from 30/100,000 to 120/100,000.1 Although it is recognized that younger suicidal depressed patients have greater executive dysfunctions than nonsuicidal ones,2-4 little remains known about the role of executive functions in elderly suicide attempts.2 Executive functions are heterogeneous cognitive functions that may be separated into 3 main subdomains: mental shifting, information updating, and cognitive inhibition.5 Cognitive inhibition dysfunction has been reported in depressed patients.4,6 Because cognitive inhibition dysfunction leads to an inability to inhibit intrusive ideation and production of negative affect,4 we hypothesized that suicidal depressed older patients could have worse cognitive inhibition performance than nonsuicidal ones. The aim of this case-control study was to compare cognitive inhibition performance in suicidal and nonsuicidal depressed older patients.

 

Method. Between January and July 2010, among the 31 depressed patients aged 65 years and older hospitalized for a current episode of major depressive disorder according to DSM-IV-TR criteria in the Department of Psychiatry of Angers University Hospital (Angers, France), 20 (65%) met the selection criteria and were included in the study. Exclusion criteria were a depression score on the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale of Depression (HDRS),7 < 18, psychotic features according to the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders,8 and a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score < 24.9 Subjects were separated into 2 groups: 10 suicidal depressed patients (mean ± SD age, 75.3 ± 2.3 years; 70% women) and 10 nonsuicidal depressed patients (mean ± SD age, 72.9 ± 1.3 years; 70% women) matched for age, gender, and education. Suicidal patients had a suicide attempt during the 10 days before the admission. Nonsuicidal patients had no lifetime history of suicide attempts and a score on HDRS suicide items calculated at 0.

Three executive subdomains were examined: mental shifting by the Trail Making Test (TMT) parts A and B,10 information updating by direct Digit Spans,11 and cognitive inhibition by the Stroop test12 (attentional inhibition) and the Go/No-Go task (motor inhibition).13 The ratio score of TMT (ie, TMTB/TMTA), the total number of direct digit recalls in correct order, the Stroop interference score (seconds), and the score on the Go/No-Go task (/3) were used as outcomes. Between-group comparisons were performed using the nonparametric Mann-Whitney U test or the χ2 test, as appropriate. Logistic regression models were performed to specify the associations between suicide attempts (dependent variable) and scores on executive function subtests (independent variable). The entire study protocol was approved by the local Ethical Committee.

Results. There was no significant difference between suicidal and nonsuicidal depressed patients for age (P = .493), gender (P = 1.000), years of education (P = .376), mean MMSE score (25.8 vs 27.3; P = .297), and mean HDRS-17 score (27.7 vs 28.1; P = .493). Suicidal depressed patients had poorer performance on the Go/No-Go task than nonsuicidal ones (mean score = 1.4 vs 2.3; P = .041). There was no significant difference for the mean ratio score of TMT (2.4 ± 0.3 vs 2.9 ± 0.3; P = .199), the mean direct Digit Spans score (5.3 ± 0.3 vs 6.2 ± 0.3; P = .070), and the mean Stroop interference score (115.9 ± 23.9 vs 182.7 ± 39.6 seconds; P = .406). Logistic regression models showed that suicide attempt was associated with worse performance on the Go/No-Go task (adjusted odds ratio = 0.26 [95% CI = 0.07-0.95], P = .041) after adjustment for age (Table 1).

Table 1

Click figure to enlarge

 

Our results show that depressed older patients with suicide attempt had worse cognitive inhibition performance, especially on the motor component, than their nonsuicidal depressed counterparts. Functional neuroimaging studies have shown that cognitive inhibition is based on prefrontal cortex function.14 Impaired cognitive inhibition reported in our study was thus in concordance with prefrontal dorsolateral cortex dysfunctions found in suicidal depressed patients.14,15 Inability to inhibit the intrusive ideation and the production of negative affects has been associated with cognitive inhibition, which could promote suicide attempts in depressed patients, as suggested by our results.4 Further research is needed to corroborate these findings.

References

1. Hawton K, van Heeringen K. Suicide. Lancet. 2009;373(9672):1372-1381. PubMed doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60372-X

2. Keilp JG, Sackeim HA, Brodsky BS, et al. Neuropsychological dysfunction in depressed suicide attempters. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(5):735-741. PubMed doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.5.735

3. King DA, Conwell Y, Cox C, et al. A neuropsychological comparison of depressed suicide attempters and nonattempters. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2000;12(1):64-70. PubMed

4. Marzuk PM, Hartwell N, Leon AC, et al. Executive functioning in depressed patients with suicidal ideation. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112(4):294-301. PubMed doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00585.x

5. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol. 2000;41(1):49-100. PubMed doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

6. Dombrovski AY, Butters MA, Reynolds CF 3rd, et al. Cognitive performance in suicidal depressed elderly: preliminary report. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16(2):109-115. PubMed doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e3180f6338d

7. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23(1):56-62. PubMed doi:10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

8. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, et al. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I). New York, NY: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002.

9. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state": a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189-198. PubMed doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

10. Brown EC, Casey A, Fisch RI, et al. Trail making test as a screening device for the detection of brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1958;22(6):469-474. PubMed doi:10.1037/h0039980

11. Godefroy O, Jeannerod M, Allain P, et al. Lobe frontal, fonctions éxecutives et controle cognitive. Rev Neurol (Paris). 2008;164(suppl 3):S119-S127. PubMed

12. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18(6):643-662. doi:10.1037/h0054651

13. Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, et al. The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology. 2000;55(11):1621-1626. PubMed

14. Simmonds DJ, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH. Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(1):224-232. PubMed doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.015

15. Neves FS, Silveira G, Romano-Silva MA, et al. Is the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism associated with bipolar disorder or with suicidal behavior of bipolar disorder patients? Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B(1):114-116. PubMed doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30563

Stéphane Richard-Devantoy, MD, MS

[email protected]

Cedric Annweiler, MD, MS

Didier Le Gall, PhD

Jean-Bernard Garré, MD, PhD

Jean-Pierre Olié, MD, PhD

Olivier Beauchet, MD, PhD

Author affiliations: Department of Psychiatry and Psychology (Drs Richard-Devantoy and Garre), Department of Internal Medicine and Geriatrics (Drs Annweiler and Beauchet), University Memory Center (Drs Annweiler, Le Gall, and Beauchet), and Department of Neurology (Dr Le Gall), Angers University Hospital; UPRES EA 2646, Angers University, UNAM (Drs Richard-Devantoy, Annweiler, Le Gall, and Beauchet), Angers; and INSERM U796, University Paris Descartes, Faculty of Medicine Paris Descartes, and Sainte-Anne Hospital (Dr Olié), Paris, France.

Author contributions: Study concept and design (Drs Richard-Devantoy and Beauchet); acquisition of data (Dr Richard-Devantoy); analysis and interpretation of data (Drs Richard-Devantoy, Le Gall, Annweiler, and Beauchet); drafting of the manuscript (Drs Richard-Devantoy, Annweiler, and Beauchet); critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content (Drs Beauchet, Olié, Garré, and Le Gall); statistical expertise (Drs Annweiler and Richard-Devantoy); administrative, technical, or material support (Dr Richard-Devantoy); and study supervision (Dr Beauchet). Potential conflicts of interest: None reported. Funding/support: None reported. Disclaimer: Dr. Richard-Devantoy has full access to the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Trial Registration: clinical trials.gov Identifier: NCT01333215.